I don't want to lose sight of Hansen's Trickster theory in regard to this - but one question I have about his theory is if the liminal, the marginal shifts - he notes that in times of de-structuring, political instability like when the USSR broke up - there is a re-surgence of paranormal phenomena, but the line always seems to be the same, in other words I don't understand him to say that the paranormal topics we discuss today will become mainstream some day and then the margins will be pushed back and we'll have new paranormal topics, a new margin to deal with but these phenomena will behave the same way - rather it seems by his theory the specific paranormal topics, the scope of the paranormal seems perennial. Our relationship to it varies with the structure of society. So he seems to predict that 1) there will be no mainstream academic acceptance and 2) no matter how rigorous the controls and how many replications and how strong the results, it will be rejected - even if this requires alterations in very basic aspects of science . . .
Thanks for that insight into Hansen's thinking, Steve. I don't have time to read him at present and so what you write in this post is particularly helpful. Since I haven't read him, my next comment might be off the mark, but I have a continuing sense from what you've provided in this thread concerning Hansen's trickster theory that my reservations are likely to be valid -- that is, that Hansen and others reify the 'trickster', projecting an objective reality onto this 'figure' that lacks empirical evidence to support it. I suspect that to understand what Jung meant by the 'trickster' we need to be grounded in an understanding of what he meant by the term 'archetype' in general, and that what Jung identified by the term 'archetype' is something more subtle than what Hansen's reported views of the trickster archetype suggest.
I have the impression that the application of the 'the' {as in 'the trickster'} is part of the problem, arising out of the generalizing and reifying tendencies embedded in our language. The same thing occurs in references to
"the paranormal." The more we reify '
the paranormal' as a potential 'thing-in- itself' {indeed as a possible region of being entirely separate from our own}, the less we are likely and able to think through the nature and meaning of the empirical evidence presented in
para-normal experiences and capacities that take place in the local world we live in (which, significantly, we generally assume to be objectively definable and already understood).
The more productive path, since we don't know what 'the paranormal' is, is to follow Ingo Swann's view that experiences and capabilities we recognize to be
para-normal are innate (though suppressed in various cultures and times) and can be understood as extensions of ordinary consciousness toward information available in the collective unconscious and in what parapsychologists refer to as the supraconscious. Ingo refers to these capacities as "superpowers of the biomind" in key writings of his which I'll link to.
Another argument in favor of Ingo's approach is that since we don't know what 'the paranormal' is (in terms of a thinglike substance, essence, region of being, organized activity), we cannot think
deductively about it with any concreteness or precision. But we can make progress in comprehending
para-normal experience by approaching the subject
inductively, accumulating the available evidence for various kinds of experiences and capabilities demonstrated in our species' history concerning which empirical and even veridical evidence exists.